Riddles in code; is there a gene for language?

‘I have…’

Words are like genes; on their own they are not very powerful.  But apply them with others in the right phrase, at the right time and with the right emphasis, and they can change everything.

‘I have a dream…’

Genes are coded information.  They are like the words of a language, and can be combined into a story which tells us who we are.

The stories we choose to tell are powerful; they can change who we become, and also change the people with whom we share them.

‘I have a dream today!’


Language is a means for coding and passing on information, but it is cultural, and definitely non-genetic.  Nevertheless, for our speech capacity to have evolved, our ancestors must have had a body equipped to make speech sounds, along with the mental capacity to generate and process this language ‘behaviour’.  Our body’s development is orchestrated through the actions of relevant genes.  If the physical aspects of language ultimately have a genetic basis, this implies that speech must derive, at least in part, from the actions of our genes.

The hunt for genes involved with language led researchers at the University of Oxford to investigate an extended family (known as family KE).  Some family members had problems with their speech.  The pattern of their symptoms suggested that they inherited these difficulties as a ‘dominant’ character, and through a single gene locus.

The FoxP2 gene encodes for the ‘Forkhead-Box Protein-2’; a transcription factor.  This is a type of protein that interacts with DNA (shown here as a pair of brown spiral ladders), and influences which genes are turned on in the cell, and which remain silent.   This diagram shows two Forkhead box proteins, which associate with each other when active.  This bends the DNA strand and makes critical areas of the genetic code more accessible (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

The FOXP2 gene encodes for the ‘Forkhead-Box Protein-2’; a transcription factor. This is a type of protein that interacts with DNA (shown here as a pair of brown spiral ladders), and influences which genes are turne... mored on in the cell, and which remain silent. This diagram shows two Forkhead box proteins, which associate with each other when active. This bends the DNA strand and makes critical areas of the genetic code more accessible (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Discovery of another unrelated patient with the same symptoms confirmed that the condition was linked to a gene known as FOXP2  (short for ‘Forkhead Box Protein-2’).  This locus encodes a ‘transcription factor’; a protein that influences the activation of many other genes.  FOXP2 was subsequently dubbed ‘the gene for language’.  Is that correct?

Not really.  FOXP2 affects a range of processes, not just speech.  The mutation which inactivates the gene causes difficulties in controlling muscles of the face and tongue, problems with compiling words into sentences, and a reduced understanding of language.  Neuroimaging studies showed that these patients have reduced nerve activity in the basal ganglia  region of the brain.  Their symptoms are similar to some of the problems seen in patients with debilitating diseases such as Parkinson’s and Broca’s Aphasia; these conditions also show impairment of the basal ganglia.

Genes code for proteins by using a 3-letter alphabet of adenine, thymine, guanine and cytosine (abbreviated to A, T, G and C).  These nucletodes are knwn as ‘bases’ (are alkaline in solution) and make matched pairs which form the ‘rungs of the ladder’ of the DNA helix. Substituting one base for another (as happens in many mutations) can change the amino acid sequence of the protein a gene encodes.  Changes may make no impact on survival, allowing the DNA sequence to alter over time.  Changes that affect critical sections of the protein (e.g. an enzyme’s active site), or critical proteins like FoxP2, are rare (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Genes code for proteins by using a 3-letter alphabet of adenine, thymine, guanine and cytosine (abbreviated to A, T, G and C). These nucletodes are knwn as ‘bases’ (are alkaline in solution) and make matched pairs w... morehich form the ‘rungs of the ladder’ of the DNA helix. Substituting one base for another (as happens in many mutations) can change the amino acid sequence of the protein a gene encodes. Changes may make no impact on survival, allowing the DNA sequence to alter over time. Changes that affect critical sections of the protein (e.g. an enzyme’s active site), or critical proteins like FOXP2, are rare (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Genes provide the code to build proteins.  Proteins are assembled from this coding template (the famous triplets) as a sequence of amino acids, strung together initially like the carriages of a train and then folded into their finished form.  The amino acid sequences of the FOXP2 protein show very few differences across all vertebrate groups.  This strong conservation of sequence suggests that this protein fulfils critical roles for these organisms.  In mice, chimpanzees and birds, FOXP2 has been shown to be required for the healthy development of the brain and lungs.  Reduced levels of the protein affect motor skills learning in mice and vocal imitation in song birds.

The human and chimpanzee forms of FOXP2 protein differ by only two amino acids. We also share one of these changes with bats.  Not only that, but there is only one amino acid difference between FOXP2 from chimpanzees and mice.  These differences might look trivial but they are probably significant.  FOXP2 has evolved faster in bats than any other mammal, hinting at a possible role for this protein in echolocation.

Mouse brain slice, showing neurons from the somatosensory cortex (20X magnification) producing green fluorescent protein (GFP).  Projections (dendrites) extend upwards towards the pial surface from the teardrop-shaped cell bodies. Humanised Foxp2 in mice causes longer dendrites to form on specific brain nerve cells, lengthens the recovery time needed by some neurons after firing, and increases the readiness of these neurons to make new connections with other nerves (synaptic plasticity).  The degree of synaptic plasticity indicates how efficiently neurons code and process information (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Mouse brain slice, showing neurons from the somatosensory cortex (20X magnification) producing green fluorescent protein (GFP). Projections (dendrites) extend upwards towards the pial surface from the teardrop-shaped ce... morell bodies. Humanised Foxp2 in mice causes longer dendrites to form on specific brain nerve cells, lengthens the recovery time needed by some neurons after firing, and increases the readiness of these neurons to make new connections with other nerves (synaptic plasticity). The degree of synaptic plasticity indicates how efficiently neurons code and process information (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Changing the form of mouse FOXP2 to include these two human-associated amino acids alters the pitch of these animals’ ultrasonic calls, and affects their degree of inquisitive behaviour.  Differences also appear in their neural anatomy.  Altering the number of working copies (the genetic ‘dose’) of FOXP2 in mice and birds affects the development of their basal ganglia.

Mice with ‘humanised’ FOXP2 protein show changes in their cortico-basal ganglia circuits along with altered exploratory behaviour and reduced levels of dopamine (a neurotransmitter  that affects our emotional responses).  So too, human patients with damage to the basal ganglia show reduced levels of initiative and motivation for tasks.

This suggests that FOXP2 is part of a general mechanism that affects our thinking, particularly around our initiative and mental flexibility.  These are critical components of human creativity, and are as it happens, essential for our speech.

Basal ganglia circuits process and organise signals from other parts of the brain into sequences.  Speaking involves coordinating a complex sequence of muscle actions in the mouth and throat, and synchronising these with the out-breath.  We use these same muscles and anatomical structures to breathe, chew and swallow;  our ability to coordinate them affects our speech, although this is not their primary role.

Family KE’s condition, caused by a dominant mutation in the FoxP2 gene, follows an autosomal (not sex-linked) pattern of inheritance, as shown here.   Dominant mutations are visible when only one gene copy is present.  In contrast a recessive trait is not seen in the organism unless both chromosomes of the pair carry the mutant form of the gene.   The FoxP2 transcription factor protein is required in precise amounts for normal function of the brain.  The loss of one working FoxP2 gene copy reduces this ‘dose’ which is enough to cause the problems that emerged as family KE’s symptoms (Image: Annotated from Wikimedia Commons)

Family KE’s condition, caused by a dominant mutation in the FOXP2 gene, follows an autosomal (not sex-linked) pattern of inheritance, as shown here.Dominant mutations are visible when only one gene copy is present. In... more contrast a recessive trait is not seen in the organism unless both chromosomes of the pair carry the mutant form of the gene. The FOXP2 transcription factor protein is required in precise amounts for normal function of the brain. The loss of one working FOXP2 gene copy reduces this ‘dose’ which is enough to cause the problems that emerged as family KE’s symptoms (Image: Annotated from Wikimedia Commons)

In practice, very few of our 25,000 genes are individually responsible for noticeable characteristics.  Most genetically inherited diseases result from the effects of multiple gene loci.  FOXP2 is unusual because of its ‘dominant’ genetic character.  It does not give us our language abilities, but it is involved in the neural basis of our mental flexibility and agility at controlling the muscles of our mouths, throats and fingers.

In addition, genes are only part of the story of our development.  The way we think and subsequently behave alters our emotional state.  Feeling stressed or calm affects which circuits are active in our brain.  This alters the biochemical state of body organs and tissues, particularly of the immune system, modifying which genes they are using.

The dance between the code stored in our genes and the consequences of our thoughts builds us into what we are mentally, physically and socially.  This story is ours to tell.  By our experience, and with this genetic vocabulary, we create what we become.

Text copyright © 2015 Mags Leighton. All rights reserved.

References
Chial H (2008)  ‘Rare genetic disorders: Learning about genetic disease through gene mapping, SNPs, and microarray data’ Nature Education 1(1):192  http://www.nature.com/scitable/topicpage/rare-genetic-disorders-learning-about-genetic-disease-979
Clovis YM et al. (2012) ‘Convergent repression of Foxp2 3′UTR by miR-9 and miR-132 in embryonic mouse neocortex: implications for radial migration of neurons’  Development 139, 3332-3342.
Enard, W (2011) ‘FOXP2 and the role of cortico-basal ganglia circuits in speech and language evolution’  Current Opinion in Neurobiology  21; 415–424
Enard, W et al (2009)  A Humanized Version of Foxp2 Affects Cortico-Basal Ganglia Circuits in Mice  Cell 137 (5); 961–971  http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S009286740900378X
Feuk L et at., Absence of a Paternally Inherited FOXP2 Gene in Developmental Verbal Dyspraxia, in The American Journal of Human Genetics, Vol. 79 November 2006, p.965-72.
Fisher SE and Scharff C (2009) ‘FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language’  Trends in Genetics 25 (4); 166-177
Lieberman P  (2009)  ‘FOXP2 and Human Cognition’  Cell 137; 800-803
Marcus GF & Fisher SE (2003) ‘FOXp2 in focus; what can genes tell us about speech and language?’  Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(6); 257-262
Reimers-Kipping S et al. (2011) ‘Humanised Foxp2 specifically affects cortico-basal ganglia circuits’ Neuroscience 175; 75-84
Scharff C & Haesler S (2005) ‘An evolutionary perspective on Foxp2; strictly for the birds?’ Current opinion in Neurobiology 15:694-703
Vargha-Khadem F et al. (2005) ‘FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language’  Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6, 131-138 http://www.nature.com/nrn/journal/v6/n2/full/nrn1605.html
Wapshott N (2013)  ‘Martin Luther King's 'I Have A Dream' Speech Changed The World’ Huffington post, 28th August 2013  http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2013/08/28/i-have-a-dream-speech-world_n_3830409.html
Webb DM & Zhang J (2005) ‘Foxp2 in song learning birds and vocal learning mammals’  Journal of Heredity 96(3);212-216

Gregor Mendel

Gregor Mendel (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Gregor Mendel (Image: Wikimedia Commons)

Johann Mendel (1822-1884) was an Austrian botanist, now widely regarded as the father of modern genetics. He was given the name ‘Gregor’ – by which he is more commonly known – when he joined the Augustinian monks. It is worth remembering that the idea that all religious people are de facto opposed to science, let alone evolution, is simply incorrect, and Mendel is a reminder that a keen intellect and life in a monastery (the Abbey of St. Thomas) are not exclusive possibilities.

Mendel is most famous for his discovery of the basic principles of genetic heredity through experiments with pea plants (Pisum sativum). Pea plants were good subjects for study because their physical characteristics are relatively few and simple, and fertilisation is easily controlled. Today, Mendel’s findings are ranked amongst the greatest in biology, and our basic understanding of how traits are inherited from one generation to the next comes from the principles he proposed.

In 1865 Mendel presented what would later come to be regarded as a seminal paper in the history of science, Versuche über Pflanzenhybriden (Experiments on Plant Hybridization) to the Natural History Society of Brno in Moravia, and it was published the following year. In it, Mendel laid down three main principles of inheritance:

– Principle of segregation: in diploid species each individual possesses two types of (allele) gene for each trait. A parent will only pass one of these versions onto offspring. The one that gets passed on is in principle random.

– Principle of independent assortment: during gamete formation, each combination of alleles stands an equal chance of occurring.

– Fundamental theory of heredity: discrete units of inheritance are passed from parents to offspring. Today, we call these ‘units of inheritance’ genes.

In the nineteenth century, most biologists believed that offspring inherited a ‘blended’ set of traits from both parents. So, for example, the offspring of a tall man and a short woman would be expected to produce a child of medium height. This didn’t, however, quite add up and notably Charles Darwin had struggled to account for the mechanism by which traits were passed from one generation to the next. Indeed, a coherent theory of heredity was notably absent from On The Origin of Species.

In his 1868 work The Variation of Animals and Plants Under Domestication, Darwin proposed a theory called ‘pangenesis’ which proposed that every cell in the body combined to influence the constitution of offspring. As we now appreciate, this idea was deeply flawed and would eventually be replaced by Mendel’s theory of inheritance, but it highlights just how difficult it was to account for the inheritance of traits in a pre-gene informed world, and as such, how insightful Mendel’s principles proved to be.

In stark contrast to Darwin’s pangenesis theory, Mendel showed that when one variety of pure-bred plant was cross-pollinated with another, the offspring resembled either one or the other of the parent plants; traits were passed from parent to offspring intact, not as a blend of the two. Mendel further demonstrated that some traits are dominant and some are recessive. Dominant traits essentially mask the effect of a recessive trait. By doing so, Mendel highlighted that the subject of inheritance was clearly at one and the same time more complicated and also more simple than had hitherto been suspected by theories propounding ‘blending’. Whilst it is now clear that the processes of genetics are almost infinitely more complex and sophisticated than Mendel could ever have realized, his discovery was the lynch-pin for the emergence of neo-Darwinism.

As pointed out by Henig (2000), even though a reprint of Mendel’s 1866 paper was found in Darwin’s library, it appears that Darwin was unaware of Mendel’s work and may not have even read it. Darwin may not have been impressed. He was already familiar with the work of the French botanist Charles Naudin, who had reached many of the same conclusions as Mendel but without the statistical support.

But it wasn’t just Darwin who failed to pick up on the significance of Mendel’s research; for nearly forty years his 1866 paper remained largely unacknowledged. It was not until after Mendel’s death that his work was re-discovered by the scientific community. In 1900, the botanists Hugo de Vries, Carl Correns and Erich von Tschermak independently published work within a two month span of each other that acknowledged Mendel’s research. Biologists then became interested in the work of this virtually unknown man, and the wider the paper became known, the more scientists of the day wanted to learn about him.

But it has not all been plain sailing; with re-discovery there has also been controversy. Most notably, there has been debate in recent years about whether or not Mendel’s results were artificially modified (not necessarily by Mendel himself, but possibly by an assistant) in order to fit with his expectations, a suggestion which largely stems from Ronald Fisher’s (1936, p.132) assertion that:

“..the data of most, if not all, of the experiments have been falsified so as to agree closely with Mendel’s expectations.

As is neatly explained by Novitski (2004), most scientists agree that the results from Mendel’s garden pea experiments conform more closely with theoretical expectations (with ratios such as 3:1, 1:2:1 etc.) than one based upon chance, thus arguably making them too good to be true. Since then, many people have studied and replicated Mendel’s garden pea experiments, analysed the outcomes, and debated the ratios. On moral grounds, there is no doubt that Fisher took the ethically and academically correct route by voicing his concerns, but the emerging consensus is that the criticism is unfounded. To quote Fairbanks and Rytting (2001, p.751):

“There is no credible evidence to indicate that Mendel was inaccurate or dishonest in his description of his experiments or his presentation of data. The main questions about his results can be resolved by an appeal to botanical principles and historical evidence.”

As succinctly stated by Tudge (2000, p.286) “..the complexities of modern genetics, and all the ramifications, flow naturally from Mendel’s initial notions..”. In addition, Mendel must be given particular credit for the fact that he formed his hypotheses in an era long before genes had been identified as the unit of inheritance or the structure of DNA had been discovered. He drew his conclusions from observing the evidence in front of him, and that weight of evidence led him to break with orthodoxy and challenge (albeit unacknowledged at first) commonly held perceptions regarding the inheritance of traits. Mendel laid the groundwork that made it possible for us to understand the basic form of genetic inheritance, and in doing so he founded the field of genetics which today has huge benefits for humankind; not least in terms of understanding diseases and developing medicine.

Today we would like to think that new ideas will quickly surface, and certainly given the internet and social networks all this is greatly facilitated. Even so, ideas are ideas and without the fertile soil of an open, curiosity-driven intellect they will wither and die.

Text copyright © 2015 Victoria Ling. All rights reserved.

References
Darwin, C. 1868. The Variation of Animals and Plants Under Domestication. London: John Murray.

Fairbanks, D.J. and Rytting, B. 2001. Mendellian controversies: A botanical and historical review. American Journal of Botany 88 (5), 737-752.

Fisher, R.A. 1936.Has Mendel's work been rediscovered? Annals of Science 1, 115–137.

Hartl, D.L. and Fairbanks, D.J. 2007. Mud sticks: on the alleged falsification of Mendel’s data. Genetics 175, 975–979.

Henig, R.M. 2000. The Monk in the Garden: The Lost and Found Genius of Gregor Mendel, the Father of Genetics. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Mendel, J.G. 1866. Versuche über Pflanzenhybriden Verhandlungen des naturforschenden Vereines in Brünn, Bd. IV für das Jahr, 1865. Abhandlungen, 3–47.

Novitski, E. 2004. On Fisher’s Criticism of Mendel’s Results With the Garden Pea. Genetics 166, 1133-1136.

Sandler, I. 2000. Development: Mendel’s Legacy to Genetics. Genetics 154, 7-11.

Tudge, C. 2000. In Mendel’s Footnotes: An introduction to the science and technologies of genes and genetics from the nineteenth century to the twenty second. London: Jonathan Cape.

Vítĕzslav, O. 1996. Gregor Mendel: The first geneticist. Oxford, New York, Tokyo: Oxford University Press. (Translated by Stephen Finn)